The Influence of Grammatical Number on Cognition of Bilinguals: A Test of the Linguistic Relativity Hypothesis

Main Article Content

Chatchanok Chanyeam
Nuntana Wongthai

Abstract

The linguistic relativity hypothesis has focused on the influence of grammar in language on speakers’ cognition. Previous studies show that speakers of languages with grammatical number (e.g., English) are more aware of the number of objects. Additionally, recent studies reveal that bilinguals who speak languages with different grammatical structures may alter their cognitive preferences based on their level of language proficiency. This research builds on these findings, following Lucy’s approach (1992a), to compare the cognition of monolingual speakers among members of a group consisting of thirty native Thai speakers and thirty native English speakers, with that of bilinguals (Thai-English) with varying proficiency levels in a group consisting of thirty basic-level Thai-English bilinguals, thirty intermediate-level Thai-English bilinguals and thirty advanced-level Thai-English bilinguals. Attention tests and memory tests were implemented to test the level of cognition of each participant, the results being analyzed using ANOVA and Scheffe’s test. The results showed that the English-speaking subjects paid more attention to the number of objects and memorized a greater number of them than the Thai-speaking subjects, suggesting that the presence of grammatical number in English and its absence in Thai played a significant role. The advanced-level Thai-English bilinguals paid more attention to the number of objects and memorized a greater number of them than the intermediate-level Thai-English bilinguals, the basic-level Thai-English bilinguals, and the monolingual Thai speakers, respectively, but paid less attention than the monolingual English speakers. However, there was no significant difference between the basic-level Thai-English bilinguals and the monolingual Thai speakers. This finding may lead us to conclude that: 1) grammatical representation affects speakers’ cognition, supporting the linguistic relativity hypothesis; and that 2) bilingualism affects cognition at different levels. This finding suggests the possibility that acquisition of a second language affects bilinguals’ cognition, and highlights the importance of promoting bilingualism in Thailand in order to enhance English proficiency and global competitiveness. By highlighting how grammatical differences affect attention and memory, the study suggests language education policies should focus on bilingualism and address cognitive impacts, helping Thai learners overcome linguistic challenges and enhance cognitive and language skills.

Article Details

How to Cite
Chanyeam, C., & Wongthai, N. (2025). The Influence of Grammatical Number on Cognition of Bilinguals: A Test of the Linguistic Relativity Hypothesis . LEARN Journal: Language Education and Acquisition Research Network, 18(1), 405–432. https://doi.org/10.70730/EJVM8006
Section
Research Articles
Author Biographies

Chatchanok Chanyeam, Language and Global Communication Program, Srinakharinwirot University, Bangkok, Thailand

PhD student, Language and Global Communication Program, Faculty of Humanities, Srinakharinwirot University, Bangkok, Thailand

Nuntana Wongthai, Center of Graduate Studies, Faculty of Humanities, Srinakharinwirot University, Bangkok, Thailand

Associate Professor, Center of Graduate Studies, Faculty of Humanities, Srinakharinwirot University, Bangkok, Thailand

References

Aemdit, C., & Prasithrathsint, A. (2016). The influence of English language

on bilinguals’ cognition: a test of the linguistic relativity hypothesis in Thai speakers. Sripatum Review of Humanities and Social Sciences, 16(2), 7-16. https://so05.tci-thaijo.org/index.php/spurhs/article/view/113304/88163

Athanasopoulos, P. (2006). Effects of the grammatical representation of

number on cognition in bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 9(1), 89–96. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728905002397

Athanasopoulos, P. (2007). Interaction between grammatical categories and

cognition in bilinguals: The role of proficiency, cultural immersion, and language of instruction. Language and Cognitive Process, 22(5),

-699. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960601049347

Athanasopoulos, P., & Kasai, C. (2008). Language and thought in bilinguals:

The case of grammatical number and nonverbal classification preferences. Applied Psycholinguistics, 29, 105-123. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716408080053

Bassetti, B., & Cook, V. (2011). Language and cognition: The second language user. In V. J. Cook, & B. Bassetti (Eds.), Language and bilingual cognition (pp. 143-190). Psychology Press.

Birjandi, P., & Sabah, S. (2012). A review of the language-thought debate:

Multivariant perspectives. Brain. Broad Research in Artificial Intelligence and Neuroscience, 3(1), 50-62. https://lumenpublishing.com/journals/

index.php/brain/article/view/1936

Boroditsky, L. (2003). Linguistic relativity. In D. Gentner & S. Goldin-

Meadow (Eds.), Language in mind: Advances in the study of language and thought (pp. 63-100). MIT Press.

Broadbent, D. E. (1958). Perception and communication. Pergamon.

Chanyeam, C. (2017). The relationship between the semantic system of ‘'cutting’' terms

and the cognitive system of English and northern Sgaw Karen speakers: a test of the linguistic relativity hypothesis. [Master’s thesis, Chulalongkorn University].

Charunrochana, J. M. L. (2000). The relationship between the nominal grammatical

categories and the cognitive system of Thai and English speakers. [Doctoral dissertation, Chulalongkorn University].

Cook, V., Bassetti, B., K., C., Sasaki, M., & Takahashi, J. A. (2006). Do

bilinguals have different concepts? The case of shape and material in Japanese L2 users of English. International Journal of Bilingualism, 10(2), 137-152. https://doi.org/10.1177/13670069060100020201

Corbett, G. G. (2004). Number. Cambridge University Press.

Friedenberg, J., & Silverman, G. (2006). Cognitive science: an introduction

to the study of mind. Sage.

Gordon, P. (2004). Numerical cognition without words: Evidence from

Amazonia. Science, 306(5695), 496-499. https://doi.org/10.1126/

science.1094492

Green, D. W. (1998). Bilingualism and thought. Psychologica Belgica, 38,

-278.

Imai, M., & Gentner, D. (1997). A cross-linguistic study of early word

meaning: Universal ontology and linguistic influence. Cognition, 62, 169-200. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(96)00784-6

Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Prentice-Hall.

Kalat, J. W. (1991). Introduction to psychology (4th ed.). Brooks/Cole Publishing

Company.

Kibort, A., & Corbett, G. G. (2008). Grammatical features inventory: Number:

University of Surrey.

Kirjavainen, M., Kite, Y., & Piaseckia, A. E. (2020). The effect of language-

specific characteristics on English and Japanese speakers’ ability to recall number information. Cognitive Science, 44(12), 1-32. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12923

Lucy, J. A. (1992a). Language diversity and thought: A reformulation of the

linguistic relativity hypothesis. Cambridge University Press.

Lucy, J. A. (1992b). Grammatical categories and cognition: A case study of

the linguistic relativity hypothesis. Cambridge University Press.

Ministry of Education. (2008). Basic Education Core Curriculum B.E. 2551

(A.D. 2008). https://tinyurl.com/dc3eea2d.

Office of Basic Education Commission. (2022). Teacher guides and lesson

plans in English subject for learning through distance learning television. https://dltv.ac.th.

Pavlenko, A. (2005). Bilingualism and thought. In A. De Groot & J. Kroll (Eds.),

Handbook of bilingualism: Psycholinguistic approaches (pp. 433-453). Oxford University Press.

Pavlenko, A. (2014). The bilingual mind: And what it tells us about language

and thought. Cambridge University Press.

Reines, M. F., & Prinz, J. (2009). Reviving Whorf: The return of linguistic

relativity. Philosophy Compass, 4(6), 1022–1032.

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1747-9991.2009.00260.x

Ruthirago, N. (2011). The relationship between the grammar of counterfactual

construction and the cognitive system of Thai and German speakers: Linguistic relativity hypothesis revisited. [Master’s thesis, Kasetsart University].

Sapir, E. (1957). Language, culture, and personality. University of California Press.

Slobin, D. I. (1996). From "thought and language" to "thinking for speaking." In J. J. Gumperz & S. C. Levinson (Eds.), Rethinking linguistic relativity (pp. 70–96). Cambridge University Press.

Thongnium, K. (2017). The effect of grammatical gender on cognition of

Russian-English bilinguals compared with Russian monolinguals and English monolinguals. [Master’s thesis, Chulalongkorn University, Bangkok].

Treisman, A. (1964). Monitoring and storage of irrelevant messages in

selective attention. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 3, 449–459.

Whorf, B. L. (1956). Language, thought and reality: Selected writings of

Benjamin Lee Whorf. In J. B. Carroll (Ed.), Science and Linguistics

(pp. 207-219). MIT Press.